Skip to main content
  • Letter to the Editor
  • Open access
  • Published:

Reply to the critics on “binge drinking and alcohol prices”

The Original Article was published on 26 January 2016

Introduction

Xuan et al. [1] offer several “interpretive” comments on my systematic review [2] of binge drinking and alcohol prices, including comments concerning selection and interpretation of primary studies, alternative methodologies, and supporting literatures. Prior to addressing these issues, it is important to layout what has transpired in the addiction field as reflected in the editorial policies of the academic journal, Addiction, for which Professor Babor was Associate Editor-in-Chief. This provides context to faulty comments made by Xuan et al. [1]. Starting in 2009 or earlier, Addiction has engaged in a campaign against publication of any academic alcohol research that received industry support or sponsorship [36]. As stated by Stenius and Babor ([6], p. 191), “. . . all financial relationships with the alcoholic beverage industry are [best] avoided”. Censorship always begins with appeals to base emotions and feelings, so the campaign is surrounded with emotion-laden words that make for entertaining reading – such as “transparency,” “vested interests,” “gatekeepers,” and of course “biased findings.” Economists generally welcome open debate in the marketplace for ideas regardless of source, including contributions from researchers who rely heavily on “non-profit” financial support. For my own part, I acknowledged support received from the International Center for Alcohol Policies ([2], p. 11). All of my publications supported by ICAP contain the same declaration, and I see no reason to go beyond a simple, direct statement. Other related work that has been dormant for a decade is irrelevant and -- as I work independently as well -- did not result in research publications. That some of my conclusions do not coincide with those of Xuan et al. is to be expected in any area of scientific research. Scientific inquiry should be associated with a diversity of ideas, methods, and results, and not the monolithic approach advocated by Babor and associates.

Interpretation and selection of several primary studies

My systematic review of binge drinking [2] examines 56 econometric studies, five natural experiments, and six field studies. A qualitative approach was chosen, since primary studies report findings for numerous heterogeneous outcomes and employ different data sets and statistical methods. An on-line Supplemental File provides more details on each primary study, including summary comments on conflicting results obtained with different model specifications. It is unclear if the critics examined this File.

Qualitative reviews are assessments and not mere summaries; hence reviewers do not always agree with primary authors’ interpretation or statement of results; see [7] for extended examples. Xuan et al. [1] object to my summary of a study, Xuan et al. [8], containing “diverse” results for youth binge drinking. My tabular summary indicates insignificant results for taxes when the adult binge rate is included as a variable in the regression model ([2], p. 7), and I report conflicting results in the Supplemental File. Xuan et al. [1, 8] emphasize an indirect or mediated result through adult binging, whereas my review (and all other primary studies in my review) focus on direct or primary effects of taxes on youth drinking. The basis for my assessment was the following statement:

However, after adjustment for adult binge drinking, the association between tax and youth drinking was attenuated and no longer statistically significant (AOR = 0.98, 95 % CI: 0.90, 1.07). We observed similar findings when assessing the effect of adult binge drinking on the relationship between tax and youth binge drinking ([8], p. 1717).

In other words, taxes have an insignificant direct effect on youth bingeing, but the authors argue that “alcohol taxes may affect youth drinking through their effects on adult drinking . . . [but] causal inference is limited” ([8], p. 1718). Insignificant direct results may be counter-intuitive, at least given the emphasis in the addiction literature on alcohol price responses by youth (e.g., [9, 10]). Further, indirect effects inevitably are small in magnitude as a policy basis. Xuan et al. may have found a correlation, but their explanation for causation is not proven. When causal inference is limited, then several explanations are possible, but they [1, 8] offer only one. My interpretation of their results is supported by 14 of the remaining 17 studies for youth. Two other econometric studies [11, 12] also examine indirect effects and report insignificant direct relationships between taxes and youth or young adult drinking, but neither of these studies is cited in [8]. The results in [8] are not strong evidence in favor of a particular interpretation of statistical relationships.

Two other objections are raised by Xuan et al. [1] to studies that I cited (or omitted). These can be dealt with in summary fashion. First, Nelson et al. [13] was included as an example of a study that “draw[s] a general policy link between alcohol prices and excessive alcohol consumption” ([2], p. 2), and was cited along with prior prominent studies (e.g., [14]). Nelson et al. state that their Delphi study of alcohol policies “builds on prior work” ([13], p. 26) with regard to strength of policy evidence and by extension it reflects what is known or believed by public health researchers regarding aggregate econometric studies, such as the prior work I cited [14]. Second, my review summarizes empirical results in a 2001 publication by Cook and Moore [11], but their second article in 2002 [10] is a policy review that contains no new econometric results. All of the objections raised by Xuan et al. [1] regarding interpretation or selection of empirical results are groundless.

Methodology: meta-analysis vs. qualitative reviews

I have written at length about the methodology of meta-analysis as applied to econometric studies [15, 16], and have used meta-analysis for a number of different data sets, including several meta-regression analyses of alcohol effects (e.g., [7, 17, 18]). Xuan et al. [1] ignore these critiques and analyses, including my past comments [1719] that are highly critical of several works that they cite such as Wagenaar et al. [20]. Briefly, past meta-analyses of alcohol prices and alcohol-related harms are deeply flawed by incomplete data sets; lack of comparable quantitative measures; improper weighting of average effect-sizes; lack of controls for publication bias; lack of adjustments for non-independent observations; failure to properly employ meta-regression techniques; and other econometric problems. Further, results in [20] for “heavy drinking” are confined to only 10 studies and some results in Elder et al. [21] are based on even smaller sample sizes. Combining “apples and oranges” via a quantitative summary or using very small sample sizes are to be avoided. Qualitative reviews that are comprehensive are complements to quantitative meta-analyses, and I see no reason to view one approach as necessarily superior to the other, especially in light of methodological errors made in past meta-analyses. My statement of a quantitative standard for statistical significance was carefully explained in my review ([2], p. 5–6).

Supporting studies: controlled experiments vs. natural experiments

It comes as a surprise that Xuan et al. [1] seek additional evidence from laboratory experiments, but cite only two old “controlled” experiments by Babor and associates that use the same small sample of 34 volunteer drinkers. No interrupted time-series (ARIMA) studies are cited or other natural experiments. However, Babor and associates state the following about advantages of different primary studies, data collections, and methodologies:

“Studies of what happens when there is a change – an implemented or discontinued intervention – provide the most valuable evidence on the effects of alcohol policy . . . the various modes of data collection have different advantages and drawbacks . . . [but] in general, as one moves from individual-level to population-level interventions, the utility of [controlled] experimental methods becomes problematic. One reason is that controlled experiments may not be the best way of evaluating behaviours and outcomes in complex real-life settings ([14], p. 105).

Xuan et al. [1] could have instead chosen to review natural or quasi-experiments, including those that connect alcohol prices to alcohol harms in real-life settings. Recently, I have completed a systematic review of 55 studies of natural experiments for interventions involving alcohol prices in four European countries and Hong Kong [Nelson JP. What happens to drinking and harms when alcohol policy changes? A systematic review of five natural experiments for alcohol taxes, prices, and availability. Unpublished SSRN Working Paper 2612580. 2015]. These studies contain 78 results for alcohol consumption or harms and alcohol taxes. A number of ARIMA studies also are included. Results for binge drinking in this review support my conclusions in [2] as well as more selective results in my other reviews in this area [22, 23]. This is important in light of the fact that a population-level approach to alcohol policy is favored by public health researchers, including my critics. Binge drinking is not a trivial public health problem, nor did I suggest that it was, but the critics do a disservice to other researchers and policymakers by pretending to be informed authorities on all counts in their faulty set of comments.

References

  1. Xuan Z, Babor TF, Naimi TS, Blanchette JG, Chaloupka FJ. Comment on "binge drinking and alcohol prices." Health Econ Rev. 2016;6:4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Nelson JP. Binge drinking and alcohol prices: a systematic review of age-related results from econometric studies, natural experiments and field studies. Health Econ Rev. 2015;5:6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Babor TF. Alcohol research and the alcoholic beverage industry: issues, concerns and conflicts. Addiction. 2009;104 Suppl 1:34–47.

  4. Babor T, Miller P, Edwards G. Vested interests, addiction research and public policy. Addiction. 2010;105:4–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Babor TF, Miller PG. McCarthyism, conflict of interest and Addiction’s new transparency declaration procedures. Addiction. 2014;109:341–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stenius K, Babor TF. The alcohol industry and public interest science. Addiction. 2009;105:191–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Nelson JP. Alcohol marketing, adolescent drinking and publication bias in longitudinal studies: a critical survey using meta-analysis. J Econ Surveys. 2010;25:191–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Xuan Z, Nelson T, Heeren T, Blanchette J, Nelson D, Gruenewald P, et al. Tax policy, adults binge drinking, and youth alcohol consumption in the United States. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37:1713–9.

  9. Xu X, Chaloupka FJ. The effects of prices on alcohol use and its consequences. Alcohol Res Health. 2011;34:236–45.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Cook PJ, Moore MJ. The economics of alcohol abuse and alcohol-control policies. Health Affairs. 2002;21:120–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Cook PJ, Moore MJ. Environment and persistence in youthful drinking patterns. In: Gruber J, editor. Risky Behavior among Youths: An Economic Analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2001. p. 375–437.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  12. Nelson JP. How similar are youth and alcohol behaviors? Panel results for excise taxes and outlet density. Atl Econ J. 2008;36:89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Nelson TF, Xuan Z, Babor TF, Brewer RD, Chaloupka FJ, Gruenewald PJ, et al. Efficacy and the strength of evidence of U.S. alcohol control policies. Amer J Prev Med. 2013;445:19–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Babor T, Caetano R, Cassswell S, Edwards G, Giesbrecht N, Graham K, et al. Alcohol: No Ordinary Commodity – Research and Public Policy. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  15. Nelson JP, Kennedy PE. The use (and abuse) of meta-analysis in environmental and resource economics: an assessment. Env Res Econ. 2009;42:345–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Nelson JP. Meta-analysis: statistical methods. In: Johnston RJ, Rolfe J, Rosenberger RS, Brouwer R, editors. Benefit Transfer of Environmental and Resource Values: A Guide for Researchers and Practitioners. New York: Springer; 2015. p. 329–56.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  17. Nelson JP. Meta-analysis of alcohol prices and income elasticities – with corrections for publication bias. Health Econ Rev. 2013;3:17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Nelson JP. Estimating the price elasticity of beer: meta-analysis of data with heterogeneity, dependence, and publication bias. J Health Econ. 2014;33:180–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nelson JP. Reply to Ludbrook, Holmes and Stockwell: gender differences in alcohol demand. Health Econ. 2014;23:1284–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wagenaar AC, Salois MJ, Komro KA. Effects of beverage alcohol price and tax levels on drinking: a meta-analysis of 1003 estimates from 112 studies. Addiction. 2009;104:179–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Elder RW, Lawerence B, Ferguson A, Naimi TS, Brewer RD, Chattopadhyay SK, et al. The effectiveness of tax policy interventions for reducing excessive alcohol consumption related harms. Amer J Prev Med. 2010;38:217–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Nelson JP. Does heavy drinking by adults respond to higher alcohol prices and taxes? A survey and assessment. Econ Analysis Pol. 2013;43:265–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Nelson JP. Gender differences in alcohol demand: a systematic review of the role of prices and taxes. Health Econ. 2014;23:1260–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jon P. Nelson.

Additional information

Competing interests

No financial support was received in connection with this reply. This paper presents the work product, findings, viewpoints, and conclusions solely of the author. As with the initial paper and all other published studies, this reply was not subject to review or approval by the International Alliance for Responsible Drinking or any of IARD’s sponsoring companies.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nelson, J.P. Reply to the critics on “binge drinking and alcohol prices”. Health Econ Rev 6, 6 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-016-0084-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-016-0084-8

Keywords