Skip to main content

Attributes in stated preference elicitation studies on colorectal cancer screening and their relative importance for decision-making among screenees: a systematic review

Abstract

Introduction

The SIGMO study (Sigmoidoscopy as an evidence-based colorectal cancer screening test – a possible option?) examines screening eligible populations’ preferences for colorectal cancer (CRC) screening in Germany using a discrete choice experiment (DCE). Attribute identification and selection are essential for the construction of choice tasks and should be evidence-based. As a part of the SIGMO study this systematic review provides an overview of attributes included in studies eliciting stated preferences for CRC screening tests and their relative importance for decision-making.

Methods

Systematic search (November 2021) for English-language studies published since January 2000 in PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, Biomedical Reference Collection: Corporate Edition, LIVIVO and PsycINFO. DCEs and conjoint analysis ranking or rating tasks on screening eligible populations’ preferences for stool testing, sigmoidoscopy, and/or colonoscopy were included. Attributes were extracted and their relative importance was calculated and ranked. Risk of bias (RoB) of included studies was assessed using a modified GRADE (Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation) approach. Study selection and RoB rating were carried out independently by two reviewers. Data were extracted by one reviewer and checked by another one.

Results

A total of 23 publications on 22 studies were included. Overall RoB was rated as serious/critical for 21 studies and as moderate for 2 studies. Main reasons for high RoB were non-random sampling, low response rates, lack of non-responder analyses, and, to a lesser extent, weaknesses in the measurement instrument and data analysis. Extracted attributes (n = 120) referred to procedure-related characteristics (n = 42; 35%), structural characteristics of health care (n = 24; 20%), test characteristics (n = 23; 19%), harms (n = 16; 13%), benefits (n = 13; 11%), and level of evidence (n = 2; 2%). Most important attributes were reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) (n = 7), test sensitivity (n = 7), out-of-pocket costs (n = 4), procedure (n = 3), and frequency (n = 2).

Conclusions

Health preference studies on CRC were found to have a high RoB. The composition of choice tasks revealed a lack of attributes on patient-important outcomes (like incidence reduction), while attributes not considered relevant for individual screening decisions (like sensitivity) were frequently used. Future studies eliciting stated preferences in cancer screening should apply the principles of informed decision-making in attribute identification and selection.

Introduction

Limited health care resources require prioritisation or rationing of health services and goods [1,2,3]. Regulatory decisions on health care (e.g., reimbursement decisions) should take into account the needs and preferences of patients and the public as potential beneficiaries [3, 4]. The U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) [5] notes that considering patient preferences is particularly important in preference-sensitive decisions where multiple treatment options are available but none is clearly superior to the others for all patients. To elicit preferences in the context of the benefit-risk assessment of health services or goods, the FDA [5] and the German Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care [6] recommend, among others, stated preference methods such as the choice-based conjoint analysis (CA), also known as discrete choice experiment (DCE).

A DCE is a multi-attribute preference elicitation method [7]. The respondents are usually presented with several choice tasks (choice sets), each comparing two or more (hypothetical) alternatives. In each choice set, respondents are asked to choose the alternative they most prefer. The alternatives are defined by several attributes (e.g., frequency of screening test) with different levels (e.g., every year – every 5 years – every 10 years) assigned to each of them [2, 7, 8]. Based on the choices made, the relative importance of the attributes, trade-offs between them and the predicted uptake of health services or products can be determined [4, 9].

Within preference elicitation research, the choice of colorectal cancer (CRC) screening is becoming increasingly recognized (e.g., [10, 11]). CRC was the third most commonly diagnosed cancer and the second leading cause of cancer deaths worldwide in 2020 [12]. Recommendations for CRC screening differ between countries. The most frequently recommended screening methods are, however, faecal occult blood testing (FOBT), either guaiac-based (gFOBT) or more recently immunochemical (FIT), flexible sigmoidoscopy and colonoscopy [13,14,15,16,17]. In Germany, everyone who is eligible for screening within the statutory health insurance according to age can decide between FIT and colonoscopy as part of an organised, quality-assured screening programme [18]. Although sigmoidoscopy, proven to reduce CRC incidence and mortality, is recommended for individuals rejecting the screening colonoscopy, it is not covered by statutory health insurance [18, 19].

The SIGMO study (Sigmoidoscopy as an evidence-based colorectal cancer screening test – a possible option?) analyses screening eligible populations’ preferences for CRC screening in Germany using a DCE [20]. To construct preference elicitation tasks, the identification and selection of attributes are an essential step and should be supported by evidence [7]. Systematic reviews of studies eliciting average-risk populations’ preferences for cancer screening in general [21,22,23] or CRC screening [10, 11, 24] have already been conducted. However, the most recent review on CRC screening specific attributes covered a search period up to April 2013 [11]. Furthermore, an assessment of the risk of bias of the included studies is lacking in previous systematic reviews [23, 25, 26]. To enable an evidence-based attribute identification and selection for the DCE conducted in the SIGMO study, as well as for future stated preference elicitation studies related to CRC screening, the objectives of this systematic review were to 1) provide an overview of the attributes that have been included in CAs or DCEs eliciting screening eligible populations’ preferences for CRC screening tests, and 2) to analyse the relative importance of attributes for informed decision-making.

Methods

The conduct and reporting of this systematic review was based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Statement [26] (see Additional file 1 for PRISMA checklist). There is no separate review protocol, as this systematic review was conducted as a part of the SIGMO study. The SIGMO study is registered at the German Clinical Trials Register (DRKS00019010), a study protocol was published [20].

Eligibility criteria

Eligible for inclusion were DCEs and CA ranking or rating tasks on preferences of the screening eligible population for at least one of the following CRC screening tests: FOBT (gFOBT or FIT), sigmoidoscopy and/or colonoscopy. Only primary research and English-language studies published since January 2000 were considered. The date restriction was applied due to actuality reasons. In addition, it was not until the early 1990s that DCEs were implemented in health economics [8]. The period from 1990 to 2000 was covered by earlier systematic reviews on stated preferences for (colorectal) cancer screening [11, 21, 22, 24] but without finally including a study published before 2000.

Search strategy

Studies were identified by systematic search in the bibliographic databases PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, Biomedical Reference Collection: Corporate Edition, LIVIVO and PsycINFO. Additionally, reference lists of included studies were screened. In accordance with our eligibility criteria, the date range covered for each of the electronic databases was from 2000 to present. No other limits were applied. The last search was run on 18 November 2021.

Our search strategy combined database specific controlled vocabulary search terms with a wide range of free-text terms including spelling variants, synonyms and truncation [25] related to the following categories: screening test, colorectal cancer, screening, colorectal cancer screening, and preferences. See Additional file 2 for the search terms used and the full electronic search strategy applied to each database.

Study selection

Study selection was performed independently by two reviewers (MB and LMF/DS) by initially screening titles and abstracts, followed by full text screening for compliance with our eligibility criteria. Disagreements on whether or not a record met our eligibility criteria were resolved by consensus between the reviewers, and, if necessary, including a third person (MD).

Data collection process

We developed a data extraction form that was piloted and further refined. One reviewer (MB) extracted the data from the included studies. A second reviewer (KT) checked the extracted data; changes were made based on a discussion between the two reviewers.

Data were extracted on 1) general information (authors, title, year and journal of publication, objective, country, study duration, preference elicitation method, funding, included screening tests), 2) attributes and levels, and 3) utility values (preference weights, importance values).

We combined the extracted attributes in the categories procedure-related characteristics, test characteristics, benefits, harms, structural characteristics of health care, and level of evidence. The assignment as well as the naming of these categories were achieved inductively in an iterative consensus process considering literature regarding the recommended contents of evidence-based health information and decision aids [27,28,29]. Test characteristics include sensitivity, specificity, 1-sensitivity, 1-specificity [30, 31], and the proportion of false positive test results in relation to all screened persons. The latter comprises attributes that, for example, have been referred to as the number of unnecessary colonoscopies caused by the possible occurrence of false-positive results per overall count of people who took part in screening. Due to heterogeneity in the naming of attributes that could be assigned to test characteristics, the final classification was based on 1) attribute descriptions as given by the authors of included studies, 2) frequencies presented, which were summarised in the corresponding cells of a two-by-two table, 3) checking corresponding levels for plausibility, and 4) consenting in our research team. Out-of-pocket costs were attributed to the structural characteristics of health care and not to harms in a broader sense, as it depends on the health care system whether individuals incur costs for CRC screening offers or not.

Calculating the relative importance of attributes

Attributes with at least one attribute-level preference weight (β coefficient) reported as being significant at p ≤ 0.05 by the authors of included studies were considered in the analysis of relative importance. In studies, where various models were estimated, only preference weights from main effects models, models providing the best fit, or models based on the total sample rather than subgroups were included. Attribute-level preference weights were extracted and relative importance within each study was calculated as follows: 1) generating attribute utility ranges between the highest and lowest β coefficient of attribute specific levels, 2) summing up all attribute utility ranges, 3) dividing the individual attribute utility ranges by the sum total of attribute utility range, 4) determining the relative importance (in %), and 5) providing an importance ranking of the attributes within a study [9, 32, 33]. For studies that reported attribute-level preference weights for subgroups only, more than one relative importance ranking was calculated. Attributes with the highest relative importance values were scored as most important. The coefficients of continuous attributes were multiplied by the range of the related levels when the measurement unit of the respective coefficient was specified. If the reference value was not made explicit, the authors were contacted via Email. In cases where no clarifying response was received, 1) the measurement unit was estimated if there were any indications found in the publication, 2) the relative importance was adopted as reported by the authors, or 3) the respective study was excluded from analysis. In studies where an importance score was given, these frequencies were extracted as a measure of relative importance.

Risk of bias in individual studies

Risk of bias in included studies was assessed using the approach addressing the certainty of evidence in the relative importance of outcomes or values and preferences developed by The Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) working group [34]. We used the risk of bias domain with the following four subdomains: selection of participants into the study, completeness of data, measurement instrument and data analysis. Overall risk of bias of a study was rated as low, moderate, serious or critical, and was assigned corresponding to the highest risk of bias identified in at least one subdomain. The approach was developed to be applied to a wide range of different measurements of the relative importance of outcomes. We thus adapted the subdomains, particularly measurement instrument and data analysis, to the requirements for a low risk of bias rating to methodological quality standards of DCEs and CAs (Additional file 3) by taking into account literature on good research practice for these preference elicitation methods [2, 7,8,9, 35].

Two reviewers (MB and LMF) completed the risk of bias rating for each of the included studies independently. Disagreements regarding the final judgement of the risk of bias within the individual studies were resolved by consensus. Where no agreement could be reached, the opinion of the study team was sought.

Results

Study selection

Of 22,063 records initially identified, 23 publications on 22 studies were included in the qualitative synthesis of our systematic review (see flow diagram in Fig. 1 and Additional file 4 for an overview of the excluded records due to full-text screening with primary reasons for exclusion).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram of study selection. Legend: CA conjoint analysis, DCE discrete choice experiment

Among the 26 publications considered eligible for inclusion, multiple reports of three studies were identified. First, van Dam et al. [36], De Bekker-Grob et al. [37], and Hol et al. [38] each published different aspects out of one study. Van Dam et al. [36] and Hol et al. [38] separately report the results for a generic and labelled DCE, respectively, whereas the publication by De Bekker-Grob et al. [37] focuses on a methodological investigation of differences between these two ways of conceptualizing a choice process. Second, the publications of Marshall et al. [39] and Cheng et al. [40] also refer to one study. Marshall et al. [39] published the results of the DCE, while the article of Cheng et al. [40] focuses on a methodological exploration of different statistical models for analysing DCE data. Third, the two publications by De Bekker-Grob et al. [41] and De Bekker-Grob et al. [42] report results from one study, but focus on different content and methodological issues. For our qualitative analysis, only the publications of van Dam et al. [36], Hol et al. [38], Marshall et al. [39], and De Bekker-Grob et al. [41] were considered. The publications by De Bekker-Grob et al. [37], Cheng et al. [40], and De Bekker-Grob et al. [42] were excluded but used to complete information where necessary.

Four studies [41, 43,44,45] focused on the exploration of methodological issues related to the collection and analysis of preference data. Nevertheless, these studies were included because they contain relevant information regarding the objectives of this systematic review.

Study characteristics

See Table 1 for an overview of the characteristics of included studies. All records were published between 2000 and 2021. Most studies were conducted in the USA (n = 8, [44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]), the Netherlands (n = 6, [36, 38, 41, 52,53,54]), and Australia (n = 5, [43, 45, 55,56,57]). Two studies were realised in Canada [39, 47] and 1 each in Denmark [58], France [59], Great Britain [53], Iran [60], and Thailand [61]. The studies by Marshall et al. [47], Brenner et al. [45] and Groothuis-Oudshoorn et al. [53] were carried out in two countries each. The preference elicitation method most frequently used was a DCE (n = 21), whereas Hawley et al. [46] and Gyrd-Hansen et al. [58] applied a CA rating and ranking task, respectively. Preference elicitation tasks were labelled by Hol et al. [38], Benning et al. [52] and Benning et al. [54] and were generic in all other studies. The number of attributes included in the studies ranged from 2 (n = 1, [38]) to 9 (n = 2, [47, 60]) with most studies (n = 11) defining their preference elicitation tasks based on 4 to 5 attributes [41, 45, 46, 48,49,50,51,52, 54, 55, 58].

Table 1 Characteristics of included studies (n = 23)

All studies included at least one stool-based test (gFOBT and/or FIT) with 4 studies [41, 43, 56, 58] eliciting preferences for different stool tests only. Twelve studies [36, 38, 39, 44,45,46,47, 49, 51, 53, 60, 61] considered both sigmoidoscopy and colonoscopy, while 2 studies [48, 50] included only colonoscopy as an endoscopic screening test. Radiologic CRC screening methods (computed tomographic colonography or virtual colonoscopy and double contrast barium enema) were taken into account in 9 studies [39, 44,45,46,47,48, 50, 60, 61], accompanied by at least one endoscopic procedure and one stool test. Three studies [39, 47, 60] analysed preferences for genetic stool tests, 4 studies [52, 54, 57, 59] included blood and saliva tests, and 1 study [53] assessed preferences for capsule endoscopy.

Risk of bias within studies

We rated overall risk of bias as serious or critical for 21 studies and as moderate for 2 studies [39, 43] (Table 1, Fig. 2, and Additional file 5 for consensus answers and ratings including free-text support and direct quotations for each study). A higher risk of bias was more often present in the subdomains selection of participants and completeness of data than in measurement instrument and data analysis, the last two specifically addressing methodological aspects of DCEs.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Risk of bias rating (n = 23)

A weakness of several studies in selecting participants was a non-random sampling. Instead, individuals were recruited through non-probability sampling methods like opt-in panels (e.g., [41, 45, 47, 51,52,53,54]) or convenience and purposeful (e.g., [46, 48, 50, 61]) approaches prone to selection bias. Thus, 14 studies [41, 44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54, 60, 61] were rated with serious or critical risk of bias for this subdomain. Completeness of data was classified as serious or critical risk of bias in 16 studies because response rates were low and differences between the characteristics of participants who responded and those who did not were not examined [36, 38, 41, 44,45,46,47, 49, 50, 52,53,54,55,56, 60, 61]. The requirements for a valid presentation of the outcome including an evidence-based and justified (e.g., literature reviews, qualitative research, and expert discussions) identification and selection of the attributes and their levels as well as sufficient explanation of the choice tasks, were met by 1 study [52]. All other studies implemented at least one of these aspects. Moreover, 21 [36, 38, 39, 41, 43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56, 59,60,61] studies conducted an evidence-based attribute identification and a justified attribute selection. Checking the understanding of preference elicitation tasks involves face-to-face pretest interviews and internal validity tests. Five studies [39, 43, 48, 49, 61] met both criteria. One study [55] did not test understanding at all. The subdomain data analysis addressed whether preference heterogeneity among individuals was adequately accounted for by stratification and/or interactions between socioeconomic characteristics and attributes and/or latent class models. Ten studies [36, 38, 39, 41, 46, 47, 51, 56, 58, 59] qualify with a low risk of bias in this subdomain, while 4 studies [44, 50, 54, 55] did not meet any of the criteria and were rated with critical risk of bias.

Results of individual studies

A total of 120 attributes were extracted (Table 2). Attributes on procedure-related characteristics were used most frequently (n = 42; 35%), followed by 24 (20%) attributes on structural characteristics of health care and 23 (19%) attributes addressing test characteristics. Harms (n = 16; 13%) and benefits (n = 13; 11%) were used less frequently, the level of evidence was included two times (2%).

Table 2 Attributes (n = 120) of colorectal cancer screening tests by categories (n = 6) extracted from 23 publications

Procedure-related characteristics

Twenty studies [36, 38, 39, 41, 43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51, 53, 55, 57,58,59,60,61] included at least one attribute assigned to procedure-related characteristics of CRC screening tests, with most of them (n = 14, [38, 39, 41, 43, 44, 48,49,50,51, 55, 57,58,59, 61]) presenting one or two attributes in this category. The most frequently used attributes addressed kind of procedure (n = 13, [39, 43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50, 53, 57, 59, 60]), frequency (n = 12, [36, 38, 41, 44,45,46,47, 49, 53, 58, 60, 61]), and preparation (n = 10, [36, 39, 43, 46, 47, 51, 53, 55, 60, 61]). In 7 studies, the procedure attribute was a combination of the procedure itself and at least one of the following aspects: location of screening (n = 5, [44, 45, 47, 49, 53]), preparation (n = 3, [44, 45, 49]), recovery time (n = 3, [44, 45, 49]), requirement of sedation (n = 3, [46, 53, 60]), and test-related pain and/or discomfort (n = 2, [45, 49]) (e.g., nature of the test – half day preparation time, invasive test in a medical facility, mild-moderate discomfort, 1 h recovery time [45]). Recovery time and requirement of sedation were not included as individual attributes by any study.

Six studies had attributes that refer to the location (n = 2, [36, 60]) and duration (n = 1, [36]) of screening, mode of test delivery (n = 1, [59]), purpose of screening, which means the ability to remove polyps or cancers (n = 1, [51]), and requirement of a follow-up test (n = 2, [45, 47]). These attributes were always included in addition to at least one of the three most frequently used procedure-related characteristics.

Test characteristics

Fifteen studies included at least one attribute related to test characteristics of CRC screening methods with most (n = 14) having 1 (n = 8, [41, 46, 51, 55,56,57,58, 60]) or 2 (n = 6, [39, 47, 52,53,54, 59]) attributes from this category.

The most frequently used test characteristic attribute was sensitivity (n = 12, [39, 43, 46, 47, 51,52,53,54, 57, 59, 60]). Three studies [46, 51, 57] referred to their attributes as test accuracy and/or performance. Based on further information provided by the authors of included studies, they were categorised as sensitivity. Eleven studies included an attribute on specificity (n = 4, [39, 43, 47, 53]), 1-specificity (n = 2, [52, 54]), 1-sensitivity (n = 1, [41]) and the number of false positive test results in relation to all screened persons (n = 4, [55, 56, 58, 59]).

Benefits

The most frequently used attribute related to benefits was reduction in colorectal cancer-specific mortality (n = 12, [36, 38, 44, 45, 49, 52, 54, 56, 58,59,60,61]). Two studies [44, 45] addressed the effect on CRC incidence, but only in combination with that on cancer-specific mortality. An attribute on CRC survival was considered in 1 study [41].

Harms

Eleven studies [36, 39, 44,45,46,47, 49, 51, 53, 60, 61] included at least one attribute on harms directly associated with CRC screening methods. With 8 studies each, the two attributes of this category, test-related pain and/or discomfort [36, 39, 44, 46, 47, 51, 60, 61] and risk of complications [36, 44, 45, 47, 49, 53, 60, 61], were considered equally often. Five studies [36, 44, 47, 60, 61] used both attributes.

Structural characteristics of health care

Sixteen studies [39, 41, 43, 44, 47, 48, 50, 51, 54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61] included at least one attribute from this category in the definition of their choice tasks. The most frequently used attribute (n = 15, [39, 43, 44, 47, 48, 50, 51, 55, 57,58,59,60,61]) was out-of-pocket costs. Seven studies implemented attributes addressing information processes (n = 5, [41, 54,55,56, 59]), travel time required to screening facility (n = 2, [48, 50]), waiting time required for a potential follow-up test (n = 1, [41]) and/or whether or not test administration was supervised by a general practitioner (n = 1, [55]). Information processes comprise attributes on informing about test results (n = 3, [55, 56, 59]) and a potential follow-up test (n = 1, [54]) as well as on waiting time required for test results (n = 1, [41]).

Level of evidence

A level of evidence attribute was included by 2 studies [52, 54]. Both times it represented the strength of the available scientific evidence for the levels of sensitivity, chance of an unnecessary follow-up test and risk reduction.

Relative importance of attributes

Twenty-one publications on 20 studies were included in the analysis of the relative importance of attributes. Two studies [54, 60] were excluded because the calculation of relative importance values was not possible due to missing reference values for the β coefficients of continuous attributes.

Six studies only reported attribute-level preference weights by subgroups (n = 2, [36, 47]), alternative-specific labels (n = 2, [38, 52]), classes from a latent class model (n = 1, [51]), and framing alternatives (n = 1, [43]). More than one relative importance ranking was calculated for each of them. In 4 of these studies [38, 43, 47, 51], the rankings for the most and second important attribute differed slightly from each other, which is why they were considered several times in the respective frequency analysis.

Most important attributes were (in descending order) reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) (n = 7, [36, 38, 44, 45, 56, 58, 61]), sensitivity (n = 7 [39, 43, 47, 51, 52, 57, 59],), out-of-pocket costs (n = 4 [48, 50, 51, 55],), kind of procedure (n = 3 [46, 49, 53],), and frequency (n = 2 [38, 41],) (Fig. 3 and Additional file 6 for relative importance of attributes per study). In addition, out-of-pocket costs (n = 6, [48, 50, 51, 57, 58, 61]), reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) (n = 4, [38, 49, 52, 59]), and sensitivity (n = 4, [43, 46, 51, 53]) were most often ranked as second in importance for decision-making. Reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) was not rated lower than second important in any study. With the exception of 2 studies, this also applies to sensitivity: This attribute was ranked as less important for one of three latent classes in Mansfield et al. [51] and for one of four frames in Howard et al. [43].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Most and second important attributes and number of studies with at least one of them. Legend: Results refer to 21 publications on 20 studies. * indicates that more than one most and/or second important attribute was extracted from each of 4 studies due to different relative importance rankings

Discussion

We identified 120 attributes from 23 publications on 22 studies and assigned them to six categories: procedure-related characteristics (n = 42; 35%), structural characteristics of health care (n = 24; 20%), test characteristics (n = 23; 19%), harms (n = 16; 13%), benefits (n = 13; 11%), and level of evidence (n = 2; 2%). The most frequently applied attributes in the choice tasks were out-of-pocket costs (n = 15), kind of procedure (n = 13), sensitivity (n = 12), test frequency (n = 12), reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) (n = 12), preparation (n = 10), test-related pain and/or discomfort (n = 8), and risk of complications (n = 8). The calculated relative importance of attributes based on 21 publications of 20 studies discovered reduction in CRC mortality (and incidence) (n = 7), sensitivity (n = 7), out-of-pocket costs (n = 4), kind of procedure (n = 3), and frequency (n = 2) as being most important in decisions regarding screening for CRC.

One strength of our systematic review compared to others [10, 11, 21,22,23,24] is the risk of bias rating of the included studies using an adapted GRADE approach [34]. Overall risk of bias was rated as serious or critical in all but two of the studies reviewed, and was mainly due to deficiencies in the selection of participants (e.g., non-probability sampling methods like opt-in panels, convenience and purposeful approaches) and completeness of data (e.g., low response rates and lack of non-responder analyses). While these are no methodological weaknesses related to the construction, design and implementation of CAs or DCEs in particular, it may be of importance for the conduct of future stated preference elicitation studies. In 2011, the ISPOR (International Society for Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research) Good Research Practices for Conjoint Analysis Task Force developed and published a checklist for conjoint analysis applications in health [7]. Consequently, for studies conducted after that point of time low risk of bias ratings were expected for the two subdomains measurement instrument and data analysis. However, none of the studies were rated with a low risk of bias for the measurement instrument subdomain at all; eleven had a serious or critical risk of bias due to shortcomings in a valid presentation of the outcome and in testing the understanding of the instrument, 7 of which were published after 2012. For the data analysis subdomain, a total of 10 studies were assessed with a low risk of bias, but 9 of these were published before 2011. In contrast, 9 out of 10 studies rated with a serious or critical risk of bias due to insufficiently accounting for preference heterogeneity in modelling were published in 2012 or later. This finding highlights the need for further implementation of the ISPOR checklist when conducting stated preference elicitation studies.

Among the 14 studies examining preferences for endoscopic screening methods, an attribute related to the reduction of colorectal cancer-specific incidence was identified only two times. In both cases, the effect on CRC incidence was only described in combination with colorectal cancer-specific mortality, but not as a single attribute. However, compound attributes are not recommended, because they increase the level of complexity and, at the same time, reduce information about which of the aspects primarily drives the choices, though frequently used [7, 23]. While stool-based tests can indirectly reduce the incidence of CRC via endoscopic follow-up, colonoscopy and sigmoidoscopy are able to directly prevent cancer by removing precancerous lesions [62]. Therefore, the extent of incidence reduction is an attribute in which CRC screening tests differ [63]. Moreover, the effect on disease-specific incidence qualifies as an even more relevant benefit outcome than disease-specific mortality [62, 64].

In line with previous reviews, sensitivity was identified as the most frequently used test characteristic attribute in the definition of preference elicitation tasks and led the relative most important rating among others [10, 11, 21, 23]. Taking into account recommendations on informed decision-making, this is surprising in several ways [65,66,67], as information on sensitivity (and also specificity) is not considered as appropriate and relevant for individual decisions in (cancer) screening [30]. Research has consistently shown that conditional probabilities like sensitivity and specificity have a high potential to be misunderstood by both consumers of health care and clinicians [68,69,70,71]. In addition, sensitivity allows no inference to the overall benefits or harms associated with the test, e.g., despite a high sensitivity there will be a high probability for false positive test results if the disease in question has a low prevalence as is the case in cancers [72]. To enable informed decision-making in the context of screening, presenting probabilities as natural frequencies and providing information on at least the baseline risk of the condition of interest and on both the probability of false negatives and false positives, which are considered patient-important outcomes due to inaccurate test results, are recommended [71, 73,74,75]. Studies eliciting preferences should incorporate current research findings in informed decision-making.

Our results further demonstrate that only 13 (57%) and 11 (48%) of the reviewed studies included an attribute addressing benefits and potential harms associated with CRC screening tests, respectively. Both a benefit- and a harms-related outcome were used in only 6 (26%) studies. This is notable, as informed decision-making requires a balanced presentation of benefits and harms [27, 28, 76, 77]. Our findings are, however, consistent with the results of Caverly et al. [78], who evaluated the presentation of benefits and harms in (colorectal) cancer screening recommendations and found that 25% (n = 14) and 29% (n = 16) of 55 positive recommendation statements from 32 guideline documents did not mention clinically important benefits or harms of cancer prevention at all.

We are aware that our systematic review has limitations. First, our results should be interpreted with caution because the relative importance of an attribute depends on both the range of levels and the other attributes included to describe the respective preference elicitation task [9, 33]. Therefore, attribute importance can only be analysed appropriately relative to the other attributes within the same choice experiment. However, the reviewed studies were highly heterogeneous in the kind and number of attributes as well as in the level ranges of similar attributes included. Consequently, a comparison of the relative importance of attributes across studies can only be approximate. Second, the results of the included studies may have limited validity due to their risk of bias, which may also affect this review’s conclusions. Finally, although we developed our search strategy taking into account the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions [25] and the PRISMA Statement [26], it is possible that additional relevant studies could have been found by searching study registries, using other search terms, including non-English language studies, or publications issued before 2000.

Conclusions

Stated preference elicitation methods have often been used to evaluate preferences of the screening eligible population for CRC screening. The risk of bias assessment revealed weaknesses in included studies, particularly in the selection of participants and completeness of data, and to a lesser extent in the measurement instrument and data analysis. To enhance study quality of future stated preference elicitation studies, the use of random sampling, analysis of differences between responders and non-responders in cases of low response rates, and adherence to the ISPOR checklist are recommended. We rated the risk of bias using a GRADE approach adapted to the methodological standards of DCEs and CAs. To obtain valuable feedback on the adaptation’s applicability, we welcome further use by other researchers.

While procedure-related attributes were most frequently used in the definition of choice tasks, reduction in colorectal cancer-specific mortality (and incidence) and sensitivity achieved high relative importance by screenees. A deeper analysis of the compositions of choice tasks revealed the following deficits. 1) Attributes on benefits and harms were used in an unbalanced way and were missing in almost half of the choice tasks, 2) attributes being inappropriate for individual decisions regarding screening, e.g., sensitivity, were included, and 3) a highly relevant benefit associated attribute for consumers, e.g., cancer-specific incidence reduction, was often lacking. In future stated preference elicitation studies, the identification and selection of attributes should be based both on evidence resulting from literature reviews and qualitative research reflecting consumers’ perspective, and on the principles of informed decision-making; especially in cases where preferences of screenees are evaluated to inform regulatory decisions in health care.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files.

Abbreviations

CA:

Conjoint analysis

CRC:

Colorectal cancer

DCE:

Discrete choice experiment

FDA:

U.S. Food and Drug Administration

FIT:

Faecal immunochemical test

FOBT:

Faecal occult blood testing

gFOBT:

Guaiac-based faecal occult blood testing

GRADE:

Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation

ISPOR:

International Society for Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research

PRISMA:

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses

SIGMO:

Sigmoidoscopy as an evidence-based colorectal cancer screening test – a possible option?

References

  1. Ryan M, Scott DA, Reeves C, Bate A, van Teijlingen ER, Russell EM, et al. Eliciting public preferences for healthcare: a systematic review of techniques. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5(5):1–186. https://doi.org/10.3310/hta5050.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Lancsar E, Louviere J. Conducting discrete choice experiments to inform healthcare decision making A user’s guide. Pharmacoeconomics. 2008;26(8):661–77. https://doi.org/10.2165/00019053-200826080-00004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Mühlbacher A, Bethge S, Tockhorn A. Measuring preferences in healthcare: introduction to discrete-choice experiments. Gesundh ökon Qual manag. 2013;18(04):159–72. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1330500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Mühlbacher A, Johnson FR. Choice experiments to quantify preferences for health and healthcare: state of the practice. Appl Health Econ Health Policy. 2016;14(3):253–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40258-016-0232-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Patient Preference Information – Voluntary Submission, Review in Premarket Approval Applications, Humanitarian Device Exemption Applications, and De Novo Requests, and Inclusion in Decision Summaries and Device Labeling. Guidance for Industry, Food and Drug Administration Staff, and Other Stakeholders. 2016. https://www.fda.gov/media/92593/download. Accessed 5 Feb 2021.

  6. Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG). General Methods - Version 5.0. 2017. https://www.iqwig.de/methoden/general-methods_version-5-0.pdf?rev=117386. Accessed 5 Feb 2021.

  7. Bridges JFP, Hauber AB, Marshall D, Lloyd A, Prosser LA, Regier DA, et al. Conjoint analysis applications in health-a checklist: a report of the ISPOR Good Research Practices for Conjoint Analysis Task Force. Value Health. 2011;14(4):403–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2010.11.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ryan M, Gerard K, Amaya-Amaya M. Using discrete choice experiments to value health and health care. Dordrecht: Springer; 2008.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  9. Hauber BA, González JM, Groothuis-Oudshoorn CGM, Prior T, Marshall DA, et al. Statistical methods for the analysis of discrete choice experiments: a report of the ISPOR Conjoint Analysis Good Research Practices Task Force. Value Health. 2016;19(4):300–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2016.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Ghanouni A, Smith SG, Halligan S, Plumb A, Boone D, Yao GL, et al. Public preferences for colorectal cancer screening tests: a review of conjoint analysis studies. Expert Rev Med Devices. 2013;10(4):489–99. https://doi.org/10.1586/17434440.2013.811867.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wortley S, Wong G, Kieu A, Howard K. Assessing stated preferences for colorectal cancer screening: a critical systematic review of discrete choice experiments. Patient. 2014;7(3):271–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40271-014-0054-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ferlay J, Ervik M, Lam F, Colombet M, Mery L, Piñeros M, Znaor A, Soerjomataram I, Bray F. Global Cancer Observatory: Cancer Today. 2020. https://gco.iarc.fr/. Accessed 13 Aug 2021.

  13. Ebell MH, Thai TN, Royalty KJ. Cancer screening recommendations: an international comparison of high income countries. Public Health Rev. 2018;39:7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40985-018-0080-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Bénard F, Barkun AN, Martel M, von Renteln D. Systematic review of colorectal cancer screening guidelines for average-risk adults: Summarizing the current global recommendations. World J Gastroenterol. 2018;24(1):124–38. https://doi.org/10.3748/wjg.v24.i1.124.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Helsingen LM, Vandvik PO, Jodal HC, Agoritsas T, Lytvyn L, Anderson JC, et al. Colorectal cancer screening with faecal immunochemical testing, sigmoidoscopy or colonoscopy: a clinical practice guideline. BMJ. 2019;367:l5515. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l5515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Navarro M, Nicolas A, Ferrandez A, Lanas A. Colorectal cancer population screening programs worldwide in 2016: an update. World J Gastroenterol. 2017;23(20):3632–42. https://doi.org/10.3748/wjg.v23.i20.3632.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Benson VS, Patnick J, Davies AK, Nadel MR, Smith RA, Atkin WS. Colorectal cancer screening: a comparison of 35 initiatives in 17 countries. Int J Cancer. 2008;122(6):1357–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijc.23273.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. German Cancer Society, German Cancer Aid, AWMF (German Guideline Program in Oncology). S3-guideline colorectal cancer, long version 2.1, 2019, AWMF registrationnumber: 021–007OL. 2019. https://www.leitlinienprogramm-onkologie.de/leitlinien/kolorektales-karzinom/. Accessed 16 Jun 2020.

  19. Jodal HC, Helsingen LM, Anderson JC, Lytvyn L, Vandvik PO, Emilsson L. Colorectal cancer screening with faecal testing, sigmoidoscopy or colonoscopy: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2019;9(10):e032773. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032773.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Brinkmann M, Diedrich L, Krauth C, Robra B-P, Stahmeyer JT, Dreier M. General populations’ preferences for colorectal cancer screening: rationale and protocol for the discrete choice experiment in the SIGMO study. BMJ Open. 2021;11(1):e042399. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-042399.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Phillips KA, van Bebber S, Marshall D, Walsh J, Thabane L. A Review of Studies Examining Stated Preferences for Cancer Screening. Prev Chronic Dis. 2006;3:A75.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Mansfield C, Tangka FKL, Ekwueme DU, Smith JL, Guy GP, Li C, Hauber AB. Stated Preference for Cancer Screening: A Systematic Review of the Literature, 1990–2013. Prev Chronic Dis. 2016;13:E27. https://doi.org/10.5888/pcd13.150433.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Hall R, Medina-Lara A, Hamilton W, Spencer AE. Attributes Used for Cancer Screening Discrete Choice Experiments: A Systematic Review. Patient. 2021;15(3):269–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40271-021-00559-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Marshall D, McGregor SE, Currie G. Measuring Preferences for Colorectal Cancer Screening. Patient. 2010;3(2):79–89. https://doi.org/10.2165/11532250-000000000-00000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welche VA. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.1 (updated September 2020). 2020. https://training.cochrane.org/handbook/current. Accessed 7 May 2021.

  26. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000100. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Segnan N, Patrick J, von Karsa L. European guidelines for quality assurance in colorectal cancer screening and diagnosis: First Edition. 1st ed. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Elwyn G, O’Connor A, Stacey D, Volk R, Edwards A, Coulter A, et al. Developing a quality criteria framework for patient decision aids: online international Delphi consensus process. BMJ. 2006;333(7565):417. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38926.629329.AE.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Dreier M, Borutta B, Seidel G, Kreusel I, Töppich J, Bitzer EM, et al. Development of a comprehensive list of criteria for evaluating consumer education materials on colorectal cancer screening. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:843. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-843.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Trevethan R. Sensitivity, Specificity, and Predictive Values: Foundations, Pliabilities, and Pitfalls in Research and Practice. Front Public Health. 2017;5:307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00307.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams et Wilkins; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Malhotra NK, Birks DF. Marketing research: An applied approach. 2nd ed. Harlow: Pearson Education; 2006.

  33. Orme B. Getting started with conjoint analysis: strategies for product design and pricing research. Madison, WI: Research Publishers LLC; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Zhang Y, Alonso-Coello P, Guyatt GH, Yepes-Nuñez JJ, Akl EA, Hazlewood G, et al. GRADE Guidelines: 19 Assessing the certainty of evidence in the importance of outcomes or values and preferences-Risk of bias and indirectness. J Clin Epidemiol. 2019;111:94–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.01.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Zhou M, Thayer WM, Bridges JFP. Using Latent Class Analysis to Model Preference Heterogeneity in Health: A Systematic Review. Pharmacoeconomics. 2018;36(2):175–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40273-017-0575-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. van Dam L, Hol L, de Bekker-Grob EW, Steyerberg EW, Kuipers EJ, Habbema JDF, et al. What determines individuals’ preferences for colorectal cancer screening programmes? A discrete choice experiment. Eur J Cancer. 2010;46(1):150–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejca.2009.07.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. de Bekker-Grob EW, Hol L, Donkers B, van Dam L, Habbema JDF, van Leerdam ME, et al. Labeled versus unlabeled discrete choice experiments in health economics: an application to colorectal cancer screening. Value Health. 2010;13(2):315–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-4733.2009.00670.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hol L, de Bekker-Grob EW, van Dam L, Donkers B, Kuipers EJ, Habbema JDF, et al. Preferences for colorectal cancer screening strategies: a discrete choice experiment. Br J Cancer. 2010;102(6):972–80. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.bjc.6605566.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Marshall DA, Johnson FR, Phillips KA, Marshall JK, Thabane L, Kulin NA. Measuring patient preferences for colorectal cancer screening using a choice-format survey. Value Health. 2007;10(5):415–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-4733.2007.00196.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Cheng J, Pullenayegum E, Marshall DA, Marshall JK, Thabane L. An empirical comparison of methods for analyzing correlated data from a discrete choice survey to elicit patient preference for colorectal cancer screening. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2012;12:15. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-12-15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. de Bekker-Grob EW, Swait JD, Kassahun HT, Bliemer MCJ, Jonker MF, Veldwijk J, et al. Are Healthcare Choices Predictable? The Impact of Discrete Choice Experiment Designs and Models. Value Health. 2019;22(9):1050–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2019.04.1924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. de Bekker-Grob EW, Donkers B, Veldwijk J, Jonker MF, Buis S, Huisman J, Bindels P. What Factors Influence Non-Participation Most in Colorectal Cancer Screening? A Discrete Choice Experiment. Patient. 2021;14(2):269–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40271-020-00477-w.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Howard K, Salkeld G. Does Attribute Framing in Discrete Choice Experiments Influence Willingness to Pay? Results from a Discrete Choice Experiment in Screening for Colorectal Cancer. Value in Health. 2009;12(2):354–63. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-4733.2008.00417.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Pignone MP, Brenner AT, Hawley S, Sheridan SL, Lewis CL, Jonas DE, Howard K. Conjoint analysis versus rating and ranking for values elicitation and clarification in colorectal cancer screening. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27(1):45–50. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-011-1837-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Brenner A, Howard K, Lewis C, Sheridan S, Crutchfield T, Hawley S, et al. Comparing 3 values clarification methods for colorectal cancer screening decision-making: a randomized trial in the US and Australia. J Gen Intern Med. 2014;29(3):507–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-013-2701-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hawley ST, Volk RJ, Krishnamurthy P, Jibaja-Weiss M, Vernon SW, Kneuper S. Preferences for Colorectal Cancer Screening Among Racially/Ethnically Diverse Primary Care Patients. Med Care. 2008;46:S10–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Marshall DA, Johnson FR, Kulin NA, Özdemir S, Walsh JME, Marshall JK, et al. How do physician assessments of patient preferences for colorectal cancer screening tests differ from actual preferences? A comparison in Canada and the United States using a stated-choice survey. Health Econ. 2009;18(12):1420–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.1437.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Pignone MP, Crutchfield TM, Brown PM, Hawley ST, Laping JL, Lewis CL, et al. Using a discrete choice experiment to inform the design of programs to promote colon cancer screening for vulnerable populations in North Carolina. BMC Health Serv Res. 2014;14:611. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-014-0611-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Kistler CE, Hess TM, Howard K, Pignone MP, Crutchfield TM, Hawley ST, et al. Older adults’ preferences for colorectal cancer-screening test attributes and test choice. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2015;9:1005–16. https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S82203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Martens CE, Crutchfield TM, Laping JL, Perreras L, Reuland DS, Cubillos L, et al. Why Wait Until Our Community Gets Cancer?: Exploring CRC Screening Barriers and Facilitators in the Spanish-Speaking Community in North Carolina. J Cancer Educ. 2016;31(4):652–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13187-015-0890-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Mansfield C, Ekwueme DU, Tangka FKL, Brown DS, Smith JL, Guy GP, et al. Colorectal Cancer Screening: Preferences, Past Behavior, and Future Intentions. Patient. 2018;11(6):599–611. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40271-018-0308-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Benning TM, Dellaert BGC, Dirksen CD, Severens JL. Preferences for potential innovations in non-invasive colorectal cancer screening: A labeled discrete choice experiment for a Dutch screening campaign. Acta Oncol. 2014;53(7):898–908. https://doi.org/10.3109/0284186X.2013.877159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Groothuis-Oudshoorn CGM, Fermont JM, van Til JA, IJzerman MJ. Public stated preferences and predicted uptake for genome-based colorectal cancer screening. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2014;14:18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-14-18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Benning TM, Dellaert BGC, Severens JL, Dirksen CD. The effect of presenting information about invasive follow-up testing on individuals’ noninvasive colorectal cancer screening participation decision: results from a discrete choice experiment. Value Health. 2014;17(5):578–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jval.2014.04.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Salkeld G, Ryan M, Short L. The veil of experience: do consumers prefer what they know best? Health Econ. 2000;9(3):267–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1099-1050(200004)9:3%3c267::aid-hec511%3e3.0.co;2-h.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Salkeld G, Solomon M, Short L, Ryan M, Ward JE. Evidence-based consumer choice: a case study in colorectal cancer screening. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2003;27:449–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Osborne JM, Flight I, Wilson CJ, Chen G, Ratcliffe J, Young GP. The impact of sample type and procedural attributes on relative acceptability of different colorectal cancer screening regimens. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2018;12:1825–36. https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S172143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Gyrd-Hansen D, Søgaard J. Analysing public preferences for cancer screening programmes. Health Econ. 2001;10(7):617–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.622.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Nayaradou M, Berchi C, Dejardin O, Launoy G. Eliciting population preferences for mass colorectal cancer screening organization. Med Decis Making. 2010;30(2):224–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X09342747.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Ramezani Doroh V, Delavari A, Yaseri M, Sefiddashti SE, Akbarisari A. Preferences of Iranian average risk population for colorectal cancer screening tests. Int J Health Care Qual Assur. 2019;32(4):677–87. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJHCQA-08-2017-0151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Phisalprapa P, Ngorsuraches S, Wanishayakorn T, Kositamongkol C, Supakankunti S, Chaiyakunapruk N. Estimating the preferences and willingness-to-pay for colorectal cancer screening: an opportunity to incorporate the perspective of population at risk into policy development in Thailand. J Med Econ. 2021;24(1):226–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/13696998.2021.1877145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Bretthauer M, Kalager M. Principles, effectiveness and caveats in screening for cancer. Br J Surg. 2013;100(1):55–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/bjs.8995.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Lin JS, Perdue LA, Henrikson NB, Bean SI, Blasi PR. Screening for Colorectal Cancer: An Evidence Update for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Evidence Synthesis No. 202. AHRQ Publication No. 20–05271-EF-1. 2021. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK570913/pdf/Bookshelf_NBK570913.pdf. Accessed 13 Aug 2021.

  64. Steele RJC, Brewster DH. Should we use total mortality rather than cancer specific mortality to judge cancer screening programmes? No. BMJ. 2011;343:d6397. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d6397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Jørgensen KJ, Brodersen J, Hartling OJ, Nielsen M, Gøtzsche PC. Informed choice requires information about both benefits and harms. J Med Ethics. 2009;35(4):268–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/jme.2008.027961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Bunge M, Mühlhauser I, Steckelberg A. What constitutes evidence-based patient information? Overview of discussed criteria. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;78(3):316–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2009.10.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Joseph-Williams N, Newcombe R, Politi M, Durand M-A, Sivell S, Stacey D, et al. Toward Minimum Standards for Certifying Patient Decision Aids: A Modified Delphi Consensus Process. Med Decis Making. 2014;34(6):699–710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X13501721.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Gigerenzer G. What are natural frequencies? BMJ. 2011;https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d6386

  69. Akl EA, Oxman AD, Herrin J, Vist GE, Terrenato I, Sperati F, et al. Using alternative statistical formats for presenting risks and risk reductions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011;2011(3):CD006776. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD006776.pub2.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Whiting PF, Davenport C, Jameson C, Burke M, Sterne JAC, Hyde C, Ben-Shlomo Y. How well do health professionals interpret diagnostic information? A systematic review. BMJ Open. 2015;5(7):e008155. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Gigerenzer G, Hoffrage U. How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychol Rev. 1995;102(4):684. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Pinsky PF. Principles of Cancer Screening. Surg Clin North Am. 2015;95(5):953–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.suc.2015.05.009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Burkell J. What are the chances? Evaluating risk and benefit information in consumer health materials. J Med Libr Assoc. 2004;92:200–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz L, Woloshin S. Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics. Psychological science in the public interest. 2008;8:53–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Eckstrom E, Feeny DH, Walter LC, Perdue LA, Whitlock EP. Individualizing cancer screening in older adults: a narrative review and framework for future research. J Gen Intern Med. 2013;28(2):292–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-012-2227-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. General Medical Council. Consent: patients and doctors making decisions together. 2008. https://www.gmc-uk.org/static/documents/content/Consent_-_English_0617.pdf. Accessed 7 Feb 2021.

  77. Rimer BK, Briss PA, Zeller PK, Chan ECY, Woolf SH. Informed decision making: what is its role in cancer screening? Cancer. 2004;101(5 Suppl):1214–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.20512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Caverly TJ, Hayward RA, Reamer E, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Connochie D, Heisler M, Fagerlin A. Presentation of Benefits and Harms in US Cancer Screening and Prevention Guidelines: Systematic Review. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2016;108(6):djv436. https://doi.org/10.1093/jnci/djv436.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr. Daniela Salzmann and Kristin Tessarek for their support in the process of study selection and data collection.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This study was funded by the German Federal Joint Committee’s Innovation Fund (grant number 01VSF18007). The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data, preparation of the manuscript, and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Funding acquisition: MD, CK. Project administration: MD, CK. Supervision: MD, CK, BPR. Literature search and study selection: MB, LMF. Formal analysis: MB, LMF. Visualization: MB. Writing-original draft: MB. Writing-review and editing: MB, LMF, LD, BPR, CK, MD. All authors have provided input to, reviewed, edited, and approved the final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Melanie Brinkmann.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

MB, LMF, LD, CK, and MD have received research grants from the German Federal Joint Committee’s Innovation Fund. BPR declares that he has no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

PRISMA Checklist

Additional file 2.

Search terms used in PubMed and full electronic search strategy applied to the databases

Additional file 3.

Risk of bias subdomains and signalling questions based on the approach addressing the certainty of evidence in the relative importance of outcomes or values and preferences developed by GRADE, adapted to conjoint analyses and discrete choice experiments

Additional file 4.

List of records excluded due to full-text screening and primary reasons for exclusion

Additional file 5.

Consensus answers and ratings of risk of bias including free-text support and direct quotations for included discrete choice experiments and conjoint analyses

Additional file 6.

Calculated relative importance of attributes per study

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brinkmann, M., Fricke, L.M., Diedrich, L. et al. Attributes in stated preference elicitation studies on colorectal cancer screening and their relative importance for decision-making among screenees: a systematic review. Health Econ Rev 12, 49 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-022-00394-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-022-00394-8

Keywords